hig.sePublications
Change search
Link to record
Permanent link

Direct link
Publications (10 of 38) Show all publications
Eriksson, M. & Myrberg, K. (2023). How the communicative development inventories can contribute to clinical assessments of children with speech and language disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, Article ID 1176028.
Open this publication in new window or tab >>How the communicative development inventories can contribute to clinical assessments of children with speech and language disorders
2023 (English)In: Frontiers in Psychology, E-ISSN 1664-1078, Vol. 14, article id 1176028Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Introduction: The purpose of the present study was to investigate whether information from the Swedish version of the Communicative Development Inventories III (SCDI-III) is informative to the Speech and Language Pathologist (SLP) when examining children with suspected speech and language disorders at a SLP unit.

Method: Parents to 50 children (25 girls, 25 boys, age 30–80 months) that had been referred to the local SLP unit completed the SCDI-III. Nine children came from multilingual families and 41 children came from monolingual, Swedish speaking homes. The children were diagnosed as having developmental speech disorders (12) or developmental language disorders (33). Five children were not diagnosed with any disorder.

Results: The results showed that the referred children performed significantly lower on scales for word production, grammar, and metalinguistic awareness, compared to a subset from the norms with a similar age and gender composition. Most children fell below the 10th percentile on word production and grammatical constructions. The intercorrelation between the three scales were in general substantial. Comparisons of children’s performance on the vocabulary and grammar scales of SCDI-III, and the medical records revealed 18 cases of discordance that would have motivated further examination. The parents rated sometimes their child’s vocabulary and grammar skills as higher and sometime as lower to the medical records.

Discussion: Limitations due to attrition and sample size were discussed. It was concluded that the SCDI-III can provide valuable information to the examination at the SLP clinic in addition to parent interviews, observations of children, and various tests, and that the potential for adapted versions would be particularly high for examinations of multilingual children.

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
Frontiers, 2023
National Category
Health Sciences
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-42815 (URN)10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1176028 (DOI)001038752600001 ()37519399 (PubMedID)2-s2.0-85166409471 (Scopus ID)
Available from: 2023-08-10 Created: 2023-08-10 Last updated: 2024-05-21Bibliographically approved
Eriksson, M. (2023). Insufficient evidence for the validity of the Language Development Survey and the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories as screening tools: A critical review. International journal of language and communication disorders, 58(2), 555-575
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Insufficient evidence for the validity of the Language Development Survey and the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories as screening tools: A critical review
2023 (English)In: International journal of language and communication disorders, ISSN 1368-2822, E-ISSN 1460-6984, Vol. 58, no 2, p. 555-575Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Background

The Language Development Survey (LDS) and the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories (MB-CDI) are two parental report forms that have been productive in providing data on early child language during the past 30 years. The instruments have been used both in studies relating to typical developing children and in screening for language difficulties.

Aim

To review the evidence for the LDS and the MB-CDI utility as screening instruments.

Methods

A literature search in PubMed and PsychInfo identified 16 articles based on LDS or MB-CDI that reported statistics pertinent to early screening for language difficulties.

Main Contribution

It was found that most reviewed studies were explorative in nature and tried out different versions of the screening test, including different cut-off values, multiple reference tests, small sample sizes and rarely reported confidence intervals. Spectrum, verification and review biases were common. Moreover, no study could convincingly show that the actual diagnostic accuracy was sufficient for clinical use.

Conclusions

There is insufficient evidence that the LDS or any version of the MB-CDI is a valid tool for screening for language difficulties. Of course, this is not to say that sufficient evidence will not be achieved in future studies, or that the instruments do not work well for purposes other than screening.

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
Wiley, 2023
Keywords
language assessment, language screening, LDS, MB-CDI, review, parental reports
National Category
Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-40471 (URN)10.1111/1460-6984.12800 (DOI)000881998900001 ()36370048 (PubMedID)2-s2.0-85143211260 (Scopus ID)
Available from: 2022-11-21 Created: 2022-11-21 Last updated: 2023-03-19Bibliographically approved
Eriksson, M., Boman, E. & Svedberg, P. (2022). Autonomy and health-related quality of life in adolescents. BMC Pediatrics, 22(1), Article ID 555.
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Autonomy and health-related quality of life in adolescents
2022 (English)In: BMC Pediatrics, E-ISSN 1471-2431, Vol. 22, no 1, article id 555Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Background

Autonomy is recognized as important for individual well-being and constitutes one dimension in the KIDSCREEN-instrument measuring health related quality of life (HRQoL) in children and adolescents. However, the autonomy questions in KIDSCREEN are restricted to opportunities to influence leisure time activities, which is a form of autonomy as volition. Yet, there are other aspects of autonomy that might be related to adolescent’s HRQoL. The aims of the present study were first to investigate the psychometric properties of a scale measuring autonomy in adolescence from a control perspective (AAC) including its relation to the autonomy dimension in KIDSCREEN, and second; to investigate AACs ability to predict each of the 10 dimensions constituting KIDSCREEN.

Methods

Students (n = 154) aged 15–16 years who were recruited from schools located in both low (two schools) and high (two schools) socioeconomic status (SES) areas in Sweden participated in a cross-sectional study. The adolescents answered a questionnaire including a new 6-item scale measuring perceived autonomy and HRQoL assessed by the KIDSCREEN-52 instrument. A factor analyses was computed to investigate the relation between the items in the AAC scale and the autonomy items in the KIDSCREEN instrument. Hierarchical regression analyses were computed to investigate if the AAC scale predicted HRQoL in any of the 10 dimensions in KIDSCREEN after controlling for gender, SES and the original autonomy scale included in KIDSCREEN.

Results

The factor analysis showed that all the items from the autonomy scale loaded in one factor and that all the items from the AAC scale loaded in another dimension. The hierarchical regression models showed that the AAC scale uniquely predicted HRQoL in all dimensions of the KIDSCREEN instrument after controlling for gender, SES and the original autonomy scale included in KIDSCREEN-52. A high level of perceived autonomy was associated with a high level of HRQoL for both scales.

Conclusion

A new scale for measuring autonomy from a control perspective has been presented and shown to differ from autonomy as volition. Both forms of autonomy are positively related to HRQoL in adolescence 15–16 years old.

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
BMC, 2022
Keywords
Adolescents, Autonomy, Health-related quality of life, KIDSCREEN
National Category
Health Sciences
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-39982 (URN)10.1186/s12887-022-03607-5 (DOI)000855775200001 ()36127635 (PubMedID)2-s2.0-85138135982 (Scopus ID)
Funder
Clas Groschinski Memorial FoundationUniversity of Gävle
Available from: 2022-09-23 Created: 2022-09-23 Last updated: 2024-07-04Bibliographically approved
Nayeb, L., Lagerberg, D., Sarkadi, A., Salameh, E.-K. & Eriksson, M. (2021). Answer to the letter concerning our published paper about identifying language disorder in bilingual children [Letter to the editor]. Acta Paediatrica, 110(1), 362-363
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Answer to the letter concerning our published paper about identifying language disorder in bilingual children
Show others...
2021 (English)In: Acta Paediatrica, ISSN 0803-5253, E-ISSN 1651-2227, Vol. 110, no 1, p. 362-363Article in journal, Letter (Other academic) Published
Abstract [en]

Reply by the current authors to the comments made by Daisy I. Perry & Gurdas V. Singh (see record 2020-99156-061) on the original article (see record 2020-99156-046). We read the commentary by Perry and Singhs with great interest and would like to take the opportunity to respond to their concerns, namely exclusion criteria; quality of parental information; control for SES; limited languages; varying expertise of professionals; and low sample size. The purpose of the screening was to identify new cases. Premature (<37 weeks) children and those with known disabilities were excluded as they are already in receipt of CHC services. Although we agree with Perry and Singhs that SES is associated with the prevalence of DLD, we cannot see that SES would affect the choice between the four screening models tested, negating the need for a control group. The issue of different professionals, with unclear or inadequate training, being involved is discussed. To this, we want to clarify that the screening method was already established and has been used for around two decades in the CHCs, albeit conducted solely in Swedish with 36-month-old children, rather than in multiple languages with 30-month-olds. Finally, we are in agreement with Perry and Singhs that the sample size was relatively small and the inclusion of more children would strengthen reliability. 

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
Wiley, 2021
Keywords
language disorder, bilingual children, screening
National Category
Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-34129 (URN)10.1111/apa.15613 (DOI)000581881000001 ()33037699 (PubMedID)2-s2.0-85093530675 (Scopus ID)
Available from: 2020-10-15 Created: 2020-10-15 Last updated: 2021-03-17Bibliographically approved
Nayeb, L., Lagerberg, D., Sarkadi, A., Salameh, E.-K. & Eriksson, M. (2021). Identifying language disorder in bilingual children aged 2.5 years requires screening in both languages. Acta Paediatrica, 110(1), 265-272
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Identifying language disorder in bilingual children aged 2.5 years requires screening in both languages
Show others...
2021 (English)In: Acta Paediatrica, ISSN 0803-5253, E-ISSN 1651-2227, Vol. 110, no 1, p. 265-272Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

AIM: Bilingual children are at risk of being overlooked for early identification of language difficulties. We investigated the accuracy of four screening models for children aged 2.5. The first model screened the child using their mother tongue, the second screened in Swedish, and the third screened in both languages used by the child. The fourth model consisted of direct screening in Swedish and using parental information about the child's language development in their mother tongue.

METHODS: Overall, 111 bilingual children (51% girls), 29-33 months, were recruited from three child health centres in Gävle, Sweden, from November 2015 to June 2017. All children were consecutively assessed by a speech and language pathologist, blinded to the screening outcomes.

RESULTS: Developmental language disorder was confirmed in 32 children (29%). Only the third model, based on direct assessment using the two languages used by the child, attained adequate accuracy; 88% sensitivity, 82% specificity, 67% positive and 94% negative predictive values.

CONCLUSION: Bilingual children should be screened directly in both their languages in order to achieve adequate accuracy. Such screening procedure is particularly important for children from families with low socio-economic status living in complex linguistic environments.

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
John Wiley & Sons, 2021
Keywords
bilingual, child health care, developmental language disorder, environmental factors, language screening
National Category
Languages and Literature Health Sciences
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-33456 (URN)10.1111/apa.15343 (DOI)000564312700001 ()32869381 (PubMedID)2-s2.0-85090018031 (Scopus ID)
Note

Funding: This research was mainly funded by Uppsala County Council Grant for healthcare research. In addition, grants were provided by the Gillbergska Foundation in Uppsala, the Clas Groschinsky Foundation, the Solstickan Foundation and Queen Silvia's Jubilee Fund.

Available from: 2020-09-04 Created: 2020-09-04 Last updated: 2021-01-06Bibliographically approved
Eriksson, M. & Langeborg, L. (2021). Toward a theory of own-anchoring in judgments of other people's external characteristics. Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, 5(3), 262-268
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Toward a theory of own-anchoring in judgments of other people's external characteristics
2021 (English)In: Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, E-ISSN 2475-0387, Vol. 5, no 3, p. 262-268Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

The own-anchor effect concerns the assimilation of judgments of other people's external characteristics such as age, weight, and height toward the estimator's own characteristics. The phenomenon is related to theories of social projection and classical anchoring. It has previously been described as an estimation bias in studies of eyewitness accuracy and has been measured by the correlation between the estimates and the participants' own corresponding characteristics. We suggest that the term own-anchor effect should be reserved for cases when the estimate and the estimator's own value both are bigger than the target person's value, or when both are smaller than the target person's value. Two subtypes of own-anchoring and their association to different target persons of different ages and sizes are also described. A new index of own-anchoring based on the deviation between the estimate and the target persons' values is introduced, and differences between the two measures are discussed, as well as the implications for moderation by gender. 

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
Wiley, 2021
National Category
Psychology Health Sciences
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-35498 (URN)10.1002/jts5.91 (DOI)000631658300001 ()2-s2.0-85102750255 (Scopus ID)
Available from: 2021-03-29 Created: 2021-03-29 Last updated: 2024-05-21Bibliographically approved
Nayeb, L., Sarkadi, A., Eriksson, M., Salameh, E. K. & Lagerberg, D. (2020). Identifying language disorder in bilingual children is possible only if both languages are assessed. European Journal of Public Health, 30, 1021
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Identifying language disorder in bilingual children is possible only if both languages are assessed
Show others...
2020 (English)In: European Journal of Public Health, ISSN 1101-1262, E-ISSN 1464-360X, Vol. 30, p. 1021-Article in journal, Meeting abstract (Other academic) Published
Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
OXFORD UNIV PRESS, 2020
National Category
Health Sciences Languages and Literature
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-35106 (URN)000605268702355 ()
Available from: 2021-01-28 Created: 2021-01-28 Last updated: 2022-12-01Bibliographically approved
Nayeb, L., Lagerberg, D., Westerlund, M., Sarkadi, A., Lucas, S. & Eriksson, M. (2019). Modifying a language screening tool for three-year-old children identified severe language disorders six months earlier. Acta Paediatrica, 108(9), 1642-1648
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Modifying a language screening tool for three-year-old children identified severe language disorders six months earlier
Show others...
2019 (English)In: Acta Paediatrica, ISSN 0803-5253, E-ISSN 1651-2227, Vol. 108, no 9, p. 1642-1648Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Aim: We examined if routine Swedish language screening for developmental language disorder (DLD) carried out at three years of age could be performed as effectively six months earlier. Methods: This study observed 105 monolingual Swedish-speaking children (53% boys) aged 29-31 months at three Swedish child health centres. We compared their ability to combine three words, as per the existing protocol, and two words. They also underwent a comprehension task. Speech and language pathologists clinically assessed the children for DLD and their results were compared with the nurse-led screening. Results: The results for the three-word and two-word criterion were the following: sensitivity (100% versus 91%) specificity (81% versus 91%), positive predictive (38% versus 56%) and negative predictive value (100% versus 99%). The three-word criterion identified 29 children with possible DLD, including 11 cases later confirmed, and the two-word criterion identified 18 possible cases, including 10 confirmed cases. DLD was overrepresented in the 10% of children who did not cooperate with the nurse-led screening. Conclusion: Changing the required word combinations from three to two words worked well. The three-word test identified one extra confirmed case, but resulted in 10 more false positives. Lack of cooperation during screening constituted an increased risk for DLD. 

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
Wiley, 2019
Keywords
LANGUAGE disorders, CHILDREN, Childhealth care, Developmental language disorder, Language screening, Monolingual children, Word production
National Category
Other Medical Sciences Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-30570 (URN)10.1111/apa.14790 (DOI)000479320100015 ()30896050 (PubMedID)2-s2.0-85064834688 (Scopus ID)
Note

Funding agency:

Uppsala UniversityUppsala County Council Grants for Healthcare researchGillbergska FoundationClas Groschinsky FoundationQueen Silvia’s Jubilee Fund.

Available from: 2019-08-23 Created: 2019-08-23 Last updated: 2021-04-14Bibliographically approved
Eriksson, M. & Boman, E. (2018). Short is beautiful: Dimensionality and measurement invariance in two length of the Basic Psychological need Satisfaction at work scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article ID 965.
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Short is beautiful: Dimensionality and measurement invariance in two length of the Basic Psychological need Satisfaction at work scale
2018 (English)In: Frontiers in Psychology, E-ISSN 1664-1078, Vol. 9, article id 965Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Self-determination theory proposes that all humans have three intrinsic psychological needs: the needs for Autonomy, Competence, and Relatedness. These needs take different forms in different areas of life. The present study examines the psychometric properties of a Swedish version of the Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction at Work (BPNS-W) scale. The fit of 10-factor structures previously suggested for related versions of the scale were compared. Cross-sectional data from 1,200 participants were examined in a confirmatory factor analysis framework. Both the original 21-item version and a reduced 12-item version of the BPNS-W were examined. The General Health Questionnaire was used for validation. The results supported a three-factor solution with correlated error variances for the reversed items. Invariance testing of the long and short scales gave best support to the short scale, for which partial scalar invariance was achieved. The external validity of the short scale was supported by a hierarchical regression analysis in which each need made a unique contribution in predicting psychological well-being. In conclusion, the results corroborate a three-factor structure of BPNS-W. Although not perfect the short scale should, it is argued, be preferred over the long version. Directions for the future development of the scale are discussed.

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
Frontiers Media S.A., 2018
Keywords
Basic need satisfaction scale, Confirmatory factor analysis, Measurement invariance, Motivation, Self-determination theory
National Category
Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-27517 (URN)10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00965 (DOI)000435408600001 ()29962986 (PubMedID)2-s2.0-85048534760 (Scopus ID)
Available from: 2018-07-04 Created: 2018-07-04 Last updated: 2022-09-16Bibliographically approved
Eriksson, M. (2017). The Swedish Communicative Development Inventory III: Parent reports on language in preschool children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 41(5), 647-654
Open this publication in new window or tab >>The Swedish Communicative Development Inventory III: Parent reports on language in preschool children
2017 (English)In: International Journal of Behavioral Development, ISSN 0165-0254, E-ISSN 1464-0651, Vol. 41, no 5, p. 647-654Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

A revised form of MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory III (SCDI-III) was presented designed for Swedish speakingchildren aged 2 years 6 months–4 years 0 months with the objective to give a proxy measure of their language competence. The instrumentcontains a vocabulary checklist with 100 words, mainly predicates, from four areas; Food words, Body words, Mental words and Emotionwords. Two sections assess the child’s grammar skills and a final section appraises the child’s metalinguistic awareness. Assessments from1,134 parents are reported. Scales with adequate psychometric properties were formed for each section. Monthly median values andspread of score distributions are presented for each scale. Girls scored higher than boys on all scales. The revision, sampling procedures,demographic variables and issues of reliability and validity, are discussed. The general structure of the instrument can well be integrated insimilar instruments designed for other languages and cultures.

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
Sage Publications, 2017
Keywords
assessment, child language, language development, parental reports
National Category
Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-21427 (URN)10.1177/0165025416644078 (DOI)000407648100010 ()28890587 (PubMedID)2-s2.0-85027449886 (Scopus ID)
Note

Finansierat av Kempe-Carlgrenska stiftelsen

Available from: 2016-04-21 Created: 2016-04-21 Last updated: 2022-09-16Bibliographically approved
Organisations
Identifiers
ORCID iD: ORCID iD iconorcid.org/0000-0002-4276-7774

Search in DiVA

Show all publications